A ''stopping power'' treatise.

A ''stopping power'' treatise.

This is a discussion on A ''stopping power'' treatise. within the General Firearm Discussion forums, part of the Related Topics category; I am snagging this from THR as it is a fascinating subject - here presented by a Mr Michael Courtney - a physics prof' IIRC. ...

Results 1 to 2 of 2

Thread: A ''stopping power'' treatise.

  1. #1
    Assistant Administrator
    Array P95Carry's Avatar
    Join Date
    Mar 2005
    Location
    South West PA
    Posts
    25,484

    A ''stopping power'' treatise.

    I am snagging this from THR as it is a fascinating subject - here presented by a Mr Michael Courtney - a physics prof' IIRC. It is and always will be a contentious subject but one I find always interesting. So many myths for many!

    I offer it purely for an interest read.

    ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~

    Defining Stopping Power

    Many discussions of handgun stopping power are unproductive because they lack a quantitative definition of the stopping power concept. The issue is clouded by potentially complicating factors such as shot placement, intermediate barriers, and the mindset and individual physiology of the target. This post provides a quantitative definition of stopping power that allows one handgun load to be compared with others for a specified shot placement. The ideas presented here are a quantitative definition of stopping power of a given load for any specific unobstructed shot placement and target species, but we have in mind the specific case of involuntary (see footnote 1) incapacitation of human targets shot near the center of the chest.

    It is well known that in the absence of hits to the central nervous system (CNS), handgun bullets do not reliably provide immediate incapacitation, or even reliable incapacitation in the short time span of most gun fights (under 5 seconds). Therefore, any comparative measure of stopping power must be concerned with the probability of a given load to produce incapacitation in a specified time frame for a specific shot placement.

    For instance, we could describe the stopping power of a load as the time interval required to achieve involuntary incapacitation in 90% of the cases where the target is hit with that load at the specified placement. For example, a load that produces incapacitation in 90% of targets in fewer than 8.6 seconds would be considered to be better than a load that requires 12.2 seconds to produce incapacitation in 90% of targets.

    However, the time to achieve a 90% probability of incapacitation is not a complete definition of stopping power. The 90% probability time is somewhat of a worst-case scenario. (See footnote 2.) We might also consider the average time to incapacitate targets with a specified shot placement. A load which produces a 4.7 s average incapacitation time would be considered better than a load which produces a 7.8 s average incapacitation time.

    In addition to 90% incapacitation times and average incapacitation times, we might also consider the 20% incapacitation time, which is something of a measure of how well a load is working in a sort of ?best case scenario.? A load that produces a 20% chance of incapacitation in 2.0 s would be better than a load that produces a 20% chance of incapacitation in 3.8 s.


    Footnote 1:
    For human attackers, there is an important voluntary aspect to how hits with handgun bullets contribute to the probability of an attack being stopped. This voluntary aspect is real and important, but difficult to quantify, so we focus on the involuntary aspects for now. Our definition of stopping power is constructed with sufficient generality to allow for later inclusion of voluntary effects.


    Footnote 2:
    The idea of an absolutely worst case scenario cannot be defined with any degree of statistical rigor. As the sample size grows very large, there is always the possibility of a case that is worse than encountered previously. However, we can rigorously define the idea of worst case if we fix the percentage of cases that is better than the worst case. Here, we choose that 90% of the cases should be better than what we consider the ?worst case.?




    The totality of these ideas can be represented in a mathematical probability curve that describes the likelihood of a given load with a specific shot placement producing incapacitation within a certain number of seconds. Hypothetical probability curves are shown for three different loads in Figure 1. Keep in mind, that it is not our intent to assert that any given handgun load would produce any one of these three curves, only that curves like these would represent substantial quantitative information about the stopping power of a given load for a specific shot placement. Completely describing the incapacitation potential for a handgun load for a given shot placement requires describing the probability curve for all times that are reasonably encountered in the time span of a lethal force encounter.



    The three curves shown in Figure 1 all represent the basic idea that a good bullet in a full-sized service caliber handgun delivered with an unobstructed shot near the center of the chest will almost always cause eventual incapacitation of the target. The different curves suggest that some handgun loads might cause incapacitation more rapidly than others, and a means of quantifying and perhaps predicting this is desirable for selecting and designing more effective ammunition. (See footnote 3.)

    All the curves have the same basic features: the probability of producing instant incapacitation is very small, and the probability of eventual incapacitation is nearly 100%. The three loads are distinguished by their differing abilities to cause rapid incapacitation. Load A takes only 2.0 seconds to incapacitate 20% of the targets. Written as an equation, t20% = 2.0s. Load A also has an average incapacitation time close to 4.7 s, and takes 8.6 seconds to incapacitate 90% of the targets. One might also consider the probability of Load A causing involuntary incapacitation in under 5 seconds, because what happens after 5 seconds is irrelevant given the time span of most gun fights. Load A achieves roughly a 60% likelihood of involuntary incapacitation in under 5 seconds.


    Footnote 3:
    This is not to assert that barrier penetration and the possibility of other shot placements should not be an important part of the ammunition selection and design process. We believe that they should. However, from a scientific point of view, it is often necessary to reduce the number of variables in play in order to understand a simplified view of an issue. More complete perspectives can be more accurately built once the science of various simplified views is better understood.



    Contrast this to Load B which takes 3.8 s for an incapacitation probability of 20%, has an average incapacitation time of 7.8 s, and takes 12.0 s for an incapacitation probability of 90%. Load B has roughly a 34% likelihood of achieving involuntary incapacitation in under 5 seconds.

    Also consider Load C which takes 9.1 s to incapacitate 20% of the targets, has an average incapacitation time of 11.0s and has not caused incapacitation in 90% of the targets until 13.4 s. Load C has less than a 1% likelihood of causing involuntary incapacitation in under 5 seconds. In other words, the only way that Load C is likely to be effective in the time span of most gun fights is for the target to voluntarily cease the attack as a result of the shots fired.

    The ?best case?, average, and ?worst case? incapacitation times are summarized in Table 1. The probability of incapacitation in under 5 seconds is listed as PI(t<5).

    t20% tave t90% PI(t < 5)
    Load A 2.0s 4.7s 8.6s 60%
    Load B 3.8s 7.8s 12.2s 34%
    Load C 9.1s 11.0s 13.4s 1%

    These probability curves suggest the possibility of an idealized experiment where the incapacitation time is recorded for a large number of shooting events where the target is hit with a specific load and a specific shot placement. The data from such an idealized experiment could be used to generate the curves in Figure 1 that represent the likelihood of incapacitation within a given time. The hypothetical nature of the curves in Figure 1 do not preclude considering incapacitation probability curves as a valid description of stopping power. In the idealized experiment, loads that produce more rapid incapacitation will produce curves which are further to the left in a graph like Figure 1.

    In all areas of science, real experiments and observations represent trade-offs between an idealized experiment and the practical realities of data collection. (See footnote 4.) In the scientific pursuit of quantifying stopping power, some experimental designs might consider a variety of shot placements or use a success/failure criteria rather than a continuous variable to measure incapacitation. Other experimental designs use a small number of shooting events or perform the experiment on a species other than humans. In spite of these trade-offs, if an experimental design is clearly described (so that the strengths and limitations are understood), and data collection is faithful to the experimental method, we might be able to use results from these sub-optimal experiments to make predictions on the outcome of a more idealized experiment.


    Footnote 4:
    A fundamental aspect of this trade-off is that cost and time required for a certain number of data points scales linearly with the number of data points, but the uncertainty of numerical results is only reduced by the square root of the number of data points. In other words, reducing the experimental uncertainty by a factor of two often requires increasing the number of data points by a factor of four, which is likely to increase the cost and time by a factor of four. Various trade-offs are used to increase the number of data points without a linear increase in cost or time. These include broadening the selection criteria, studying the effect in more accessible/less expensive species, and using more available measures of the effect under study.



    Some authors split hairs by attempting to distinguish ?reliable? incapacitation mechanisms from ?unreliable? mechanisms that only contribute some fraction of the time. However, since no handgun incapacitation mechanism is 100% reliable within the time span of a typical gun fight (< 5 seconds), this concept of reliable eventual incapacitation is an artificial construct with little relevance to the stopping power discussion.

    In any case, if realistic incapacitation probability curves are anything at all like the hypothetical probability curves for Load A, Load B, and Load C, there are some practical implications for surviving gun fights with handgun loads. Even though some handgun loads might perform significantly better than others, there is no magic bullet. Short of a hit to the CNS, even the best-placed handgun bullets require substantial time (compared to the time span of most gun fights) to cause incapacitation in the majority of cases. As we will discuss later, multiple hits might decrease the time to incapacitation, but short of a direct hit to the CNS, multiple hits do not change this basic result.

    Consequently, surviving a gun fight requires more than good shot placement with good handgun bullets; surviving a gun fight requires tangible actions to avoid getting shot during the time interval before incapacitation occurs. Evasive action is necessary, and the significant likelihood that incapacitation is still several seconds away should be sufficient motive for the defensive shooter to be moving rapidly toward cover, or in the absence of cover, at least moving to make for a more difficult target.

    In summary, we have defined stopping power using incapacitation probability curves that describe the probability of involuntary incapacitation as a function of time. We make reference to the specific case of involuntary incapacitation of human targets shot near the center of the chest to help explain the concept. However, our stopping power definition also applies to alternate shot placements. Generalizing this definition to include voluntary contributions to incapacitation, contributions of new incapacitation mechanisms, and the probability of incapacitation with multiple hits is straightforward.

    Michael Courtney

    ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~

    Let me also add some material from later in the thread by Preacherman - ref' a handout he made up in late 90's to give his students in instruction courses, as it was a popular question.
    "STOPPING POWER" -

    The gun magazines would have you believe that certain guns and/or ammo possess more "Stopping Power" than others. This is a lie - there is actually no such thing as "Stopping Power". It can't be measured, it can't be scientifically defined, it can't be statistically evaluated with any certainty - in short, it's a gunwriter's myth.

    What can be measured, calibrated, etc. are a bullet's size, shape, velocity, expansion potential, etc. These add up to what I would describe as its potential to incapacitate an opponent - but even that's not exact, as the bullet has to be put into the right spot if it's to accomplish its mission. If you shoot me in the foot with a super-duper-felon-stopper-Magnum-blaster bullet, I'll be angry, not dead!

    So, let's condense the whole "stopping power" debate into a few basic rules. If you learn from these, you'll do fine.

    1. There is no such thing as "stopping power".

    2. No handgun round is a probable "stopper" - it's simply too low-powered. By comparison, common shotgun and centerfire rifle rounds are two to four times more powerful in terms of the energy imparted to them on ignition, and the energy delivered to the human body (assuming that the round stays in the body). Therefore, for any serious encounter where you have foreknowledge that it may happen, have a rifle or shotgun at hand.

    3. No round, whether handgun, shotgun or rifle, will stop an opponent unless it hits a vital part. If you strike the central nervous system (brain and spinal cord), you'll probably end the fight right there. If you strike the heart, lungs and associated blood vessels, it will take 15-30 seconds for blood loss to affect your opponent's ability to continue the fight. You can, of course, inhibit your opponent's mobility by hurting him, breaking bones, etc. Therefore, expect to need multiple hits to stop an opponent from being a danger to you. This is the rule, rather than the exception.

    4. Shock can play a part in incapacitating your opponent. This can be psychological (e.g. "Oh, @#$% - I'm shot!") or physiological (e.g. delivering a sharp blow to the solar plexus, or other vulnerable area of the body). A psychological shock can be inflicted by hitting him with anything, even a .22 - but it's not predictable, and therefore not reliable as a stopping tool. A physiological shock can be delivered by strking a vulnerable point or nerve center with a powerful enough round. A high-powered rifle or shotgun will inflict such a shock much more reliably, and more often, than will a handgun round. A powerful enough shock can stop an opponent's attack even if he's not killed.

    5. Selecting an effective handgun round for defensive use is a combination of the following factors:

    (a) A heavier bullet will usually penetrate deeper than a lighter bullet in a given caliber. Penetration is valuable, but can be too much - you don't want to endanger innocent persons behind your target. However, too little penetration may not inflict an injury sufficiently serious to stop him attacking you.

    (b) A bigger (i.e. wider, or fatter, or larger-caliber) bullet will generally inflict more damage than a smaller bullet. This can be achieved either by a bigger caliber, or an expanding bullet in a smaller caliber. However, if the small caliber fails to expand, you're stuck with a small caliber. A big caliber, even if it doesn't expand, won't shrink!

    (c) A faster bullet will generally inflict more damage than a slower bullet.

    (d) Given that you will probably need multiple hits to stop a determined opponent, you have to choose a defensive caliber and firearm that you can control accurately in rapid fire. This means firing an accurate shot, controlling the recoil, retaining (or re-acquiring) the sight picture as soon as possible, firing another accurate shot, and so on until the threat is over. If your handgun/ammo combination is too much for you to handle, you won't be able to control it well enough for accuracy and rapidity of fire - which can get you killed.

    (e) Therefore, your choice of a handgun and a defensive caliber should be the biggest, fastest bullet with adequate penetration (= weight factor) that you can control in accurate rapid fire. You can play around with these variables to a certain extent, but size does matter in handgun rounds, and bigger calibers tend to have a rather better combat record than smaller calibers. Make your selection accordingly. Remember, it's only your life at stake...

    6. Historically, the following cartridges have delivered acceptable to good fight-winning results: .357 Magnum, ..38 Super, 41 Magnum, .44 Special, .45 ACP, .45 Colt. The newer .40 S&W and 357 SIG appear to be good performers as well.

    7. Historically, the following cartridges have delivered marginal fight-winning results, although modern hollow-point ammunition has improved their performance somewhat: 9mm. Parabellum, .38 Special.

    8. Anything less powerful or smaller than 9mm. or .38 Special has not done well, historically speaking, as an effective round in a gunfight. They are not recommended as defensive rounds.
    Chris - P95
    NRA Certified Instructor & NRA Life Member.

    "To own a gun and assume that you are armed
    is like owning a piano and assuming that you are a musician!."


    http://www.rkba-2a.com/ - a portal for 2A links, articles and some videos.


  2. #2
    OD*
    OD* is offline
    Moderator
    Array OD*'s Avatar
    Join Date
    Jun 2005
    Location
    Coopersville
    Posts
    11,530
    Interesting read Chris, thanks.
    "The pistol, learn it well, carry it always ..." ~ Jeff Cooper

    "Terrorists: They hated you yesterday, they hate you today, and they will hate you tomorrow. End the cycle of hatred, donít give them a tomorrow."

Sponsored Links

Posting Permissions

  • You may not post new threads
  • You may not post replies
  • You may not post attachments
  • You may not edit your posts
  •  

Similar Threads

  1. Do you really have enough stopping power?
    By Sig35seven in forum Defensive Carry Guns
    Replies: 59
    Last Post: July 21st, 2010, 10:59 PM
  2. Stopping Power
    By mauser1959 in forum Defensive Books, Video & References
    Replies: 10
    Last Post: March 11th, 2010, 08:13 PM
  3. Stopping power VS animals
    By chains1240 in forum General Firearm Discussion
    Replies: 16
    Last Post: April 18th, 2009, 12:21 AM
  4. Stopping Power
    By ErikGr7 in forum Defensive Carry Guns
    Replies: 30
    Last Post: January 8th, 2009, 02:21 AM
  5. Stopping Power
    By crzy4guns in forum General Firearm Discussion
    Replies: 41
    Last Post: March 31st, 2008, 09:44 PM

Search tags for this page

average incapacitation time

,
average incapacitation time of 45
,
average incapacitation times
,

average time to incapacitation

,
bullet time to incapacitate
,
definition of average incapacitation time
,
formulas predicting handgun stopping power
,
formulas to predict handgun stopping power
,
handgun rounds average incapacatation time
,
handgun stopping power
,
probability curve
,
reliable immediate incapacitation is a myth
Click on a term to search for related topics.